gcanyon
7 days ago
The article seems more about the underlying causality, and less about the charts' specific role in misleading. To pick one example, the scatterplot chart isn't misleading: it's just a humble chart doing exactly what it's supposed to do: present some data in a way that makes clear the relationship (not necessarily causality!) between saturated fat consumption and heart disease.
The underlying issue (which the article discusses to some extent) is how confounding factors can make the data misleading/allow the data to be misinterpreted.
To discuss "The Illusion of Causality in Charts" I'd want to consider how one chart type vs. another is more susceptible to misinterpretation/more misleading than another. I don't know if that's actually true -- I haven't worked up some examples to check -- but that's what I was hoping for here.
hammock
6 days ago
> the scatterplot chart isn't misleading
Even leaving out the data (which you rightly point out) you are forced to choose what to plot on x and y, which by convention will communicate IV and DV respectively whether you like it or not.
rcxdude
6 days ago
True. Arguably this is a harmful convention: with any scatter plot you should consider the axes could be flipped.
ebertini76
4 days ago
Hey, I am the author of this article. Really good point! I will try to think more deeply about whether some charts induce more of these effects than others.
In any case, what is interesting is that different charts have different ways of implying causality. This is what I try to do towards the end of the article.
gcanyon
4 days ago
Now I get to fumble trying to be polite :-)
But seriously, it's a nicely-put-together article in several ways, despite my criticism.
the-mitr
6 days ago
You can check out some work of Howard wainer in this regard.
Graphic discovery, visual revelations etc.
melagonster
7 days ago
a famous example is that bar chart always better than pie chart (see the advice from page of pie chart on ggplot website).